Red de Bibliotecas Virtuales de Ciencias Sociales en
América Latina y el Caribe

logo CLACSO

Por favor, use este identificador para citar o enlazar este ítem: https://biblioteca-repositorio.clacso.edu.ar/handle/CLACSO/215499
Registro completo de metadatos
Campo DC Valor Lengua/Idioma
dc.creatorGiraldo Huertas, Juan José-
dc.date2020-02-15-
dc.date.accessioned2023-03-20T15:55:02Z-
dc.date.available2023-03-20T15:55:02Z-
dc.identifierhttps://reviberopsicologia.ibero.edu.co/article/view/rip.13107-
dc.identifier10.33881/2027-1786.rip.13107-
dc.identifier.urihttps://biblioteca-repositorio.clacso.edu.ar/handle/CLACSO/215499-
dc.descriptionChild development in the framework of the life-cycle may be limited if variables are chosen, in addition to age, and limited to inevitable conditions for survival. This article presents a broad definition of development, goes beyond the life-cycle framework and presents an evolutionary structure of care, providing evidence of the importance of considering daily and contingent activities. For this purpose, in addition to a previous conceptual discussion on an evolutionary structure of socio-cognitive care and development, some results of the Equal Start of Life Program (Inicio Parejo de la Vida in Spanish) was analyzed and carried out with a cross-sectional design and a representative population sample of two regions of Colombia: 15 municipalities in the Sabana Centro de Cundinamarca area and five municipalities in Boyacá. The data of 1177 primary caregiver-child pairs is analyzed when responding to surveys on reading, playing and other activities considered to be constitutive and indispensable in the adopted definition of child development and care. The results indicate a significant relationship between care variables and observation of the development of boys and girls between 0 and 6 years old (F(37, 229) = 26.27, p <.0001, R 2 = .15). An adverse situation is found for the potential of individual development for more than 80% of the participants, and two hypotheses are generated to discuss and continue in future controlled studies: neglect of daily reading, play, painting and sports activities, among others. , how interactions between caregivers and children, plus another as an alternative and within the studies of cognitive scarcity (Scarcity) reported in other topics related to poverty and its effects on development.en-US
dc.descriptionEl desarrollo infantil en el marco del ciclo vital puede resultar limitado si se eligen variables, además de la edad, que sólo se circunscriben a condiciones inevitables para la supervivencia. Este artículo recoge un intento por lograr una definición amplia, más allá del ciclo vital y en el marco de una estructura evolutiva del cuidado. Para tal objetivo, además de una discusión conceptual previa sobre una estructura evolutiva del cuidado y desarrollo socio-cognitivo, se presentan algunos resultados del Programa Inicio Parejo de la Vida. Se analizan los datos de 1177 participantes sobre actividades delectura, juego y otras más consideradas constitutivas e indispensables en la definición adoptada de desarrollo infantil y cuidado. Un primer análisis descriptivo de estos resultados indica una situación adversa para más del 80% de los niños participantes y genera dos hipótesis a discutir e indica una necesidad de atender con mayor urgencia la función de los agentes de cuidado y desarrollo en diversos niveles de atención.es-ES
dc.formatapplication/pdf-
dc.languagespa-
dc.publisherĬbērAMes-ES
dc.relationhttps://reviberopsicologia.ibero.edu.co/article/view/rip.13107/1568-
dc.relation/*ref*/Adolfsson, M. (2011). Applying the ICF-CY to identify everyday life situations of children and youth with disabilities. Jönköping University. Jönköping. Recuperado de: https://pdfs.semanticscholar.org/43e6/5cdae239b77ff059f80bbef80aa69a00e38c.pdf-
dc.relation/*ref*/Allen, G. (2011). Early Intervention: The Next Steps. Review Literature And Arts Of The Americas, (January), 164. Recuperado de: http://www.dwp.gov.uk/docs/early-intervention-next-steps.pdf-
dc.relation/*ref*/Appelbaum, M., Cooper, H., Kline, R. B., Mayo-Wilson, E., Nezu, A. M., & Rao, S. M. (2018). Journal article reporting standards for quantitative research in psychology: The APA Publications and Communications Board task force report. American Psychologist, 73(1), 3-25. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/amp0000191-
dc.relation/*ref*/Baltes, P. & Smith, J. (2003). New frontiers in the future of aging: From successful aging of the young old to the dilemmas of the fourth age. Gerontology, 49, 123–135.-
dc.relation/*ref*/Baltes, P., Lindenberger, U., & Staudinger, U. M. (1998). Life-span theory in developmental psychology. En: R. M. Lerner (Ed.), Handbook of child psychology: Vol. 1. Theoretical models of human development (5th ed., pp. 1029 –1143). New York: Wiley-
dc.relation/*ref*/Bandura, A. (1986). Social foundations of thought and action. New Jersey: Prentice-Hall.-
dc.relation/*ref*/Bernal, R., Attanasio, O., Peña, X. & Vera-Hernández, M. (2015). Efectos de la Transición de Educación Inicial Comunitaria a Atención en Centros de Desarrollo Infantil en Colombia, Documentos Cede 014067, Universidad de los Andes-Cede: Bogota, D.C. Recuperado de: https://ideas.repec.org/p/col/000089/014067.html-
dc.relation/*ref*/Biedinger, N. (2011). The influence of education and home environment on the cognitive outcomes of preschool children in Germany. Child Development Research, 1-10. doi:10.1155/2011/916303-
dc.relation/*ref*/Black, M., Walker, S., Fernald, L., Andersen, C., DiGirolamo, A., Lu, C., McCoy, D., Fink, G. Shawar, Y., Shiffman, J. … Grantham-McGregor, S. (2016). Advancing early childhood development: From science to scale 1 - early childhood development coming of age: science through the life course. The Lancet, doi: 10.1016/ S0140-6736(16)31389-7-
dc.relation/*ref*/Bornstein, M., Putnick, D., Oburu, P., Lansford, J., Deater-Deckard, K., Bradley, R., …, & Britto, P. (2017). Parenting, environment, and early child development in Sub-Saharan Africa. En: Abubakar, A., & Vijver, F. J. R. (Eds.). Handbook of applied developmental science in Sub-Saharan Africa. New York: Springer.-
dc.relation/*ref*/Bradley, R. & Corwyn, R. (2002). Socioeconomic status and child development. Annual Review of-
dc.relation/*ref*/Psychology, 53, 371–399.-
dc.relation/*ref*/Callaghan, T., Moll, H., Rakoczy, H., Warneken, F., Liszkowski, U., Behne, T., & Tomasello, M. (2011). Early social cognición in three cultural contexts. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 76(2), 1-142. doi:10.1111/j.1540-5834.2011.00603.x-
dc.relation/*ref*/Camerer, C., Dreber, A., Holzmeister, F., Ho, T.-H., Huber, J., Johannesson, M., ... Wu, H. (2018). Evaluating the replicability of social science experiments in Nature and Science between 2010 and 2015. Nature Human Behavior, 2, 637–644. doi: 10.1126/science.aaf0918-
dc.relation/*ref*/Colby, A. y Kohlberg, L. (1987). The measurement of moral judgment. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.-
dc.relation/*ref*/Cuadras, C. (2019). Nuevos Métodos de Análisis Multivariante. CMC Editions: Barcelona.-
dc.relation/*ref*/Cuartas, J., & Rey-Guerra, C. (2019). Ecological predictors of perceiving scarcity in childhood. Child Indicators Research, 12(1), 99-113.https://doi.org/10.1007/s12187-017-9503-6-
dc.relation/*ref*/Diaz, A. (2016). Factores asociados al desarrollo socio-cognitivo de la primera infancia en Cundinamarca y Boyacá. (Unpublished Masther’s Thesis). Universidad de los Andes, Colombia.-
dc.relation/*ref*/Edmunds, C. (2020). Academic failure and the role of early life course economic deprivation. Children and Youth Services Review, 108, 104528. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.childyouth.2019.104528.-
dc.relation/*ref*/Fay-Stammbach, T., Hawes, D. & Meredith, P. (2014). Parenting influences on executive function in early childhood: A review. Child Development Perspectives, 8(4), 258-264. doi: 10.1111/cdep.12095-
dc.relation/*ref*/Frankenburg, W. (1987). Revised Denver Pre-screening Developmental Questionnaire (PDQII). Denver, CO: DDM, Inc.-
dc.relation/*ref*/Frankenburg, W., van Doorninck, W., Liddell, T. & Dick, N. (1976). The Denver Prescreening Developmental Questionnaire (PDQ). Pediatrics, 57(5), 744-753. Recuperado de: https://pediatrics.aappublications.org/content/57/5/744.long-
dc.relation/*ref*/Fuentes-Biggi, J., Fernandez, I. & Alvarez, E. (1992). Escalas Haizea-Llevant para la evaluación del desarrollo de 0 a 6 años. Generalitat de Cataluña: Vitoria.-
dc.relation/*ref*/Gardner, J., Walker, S., Powell, C. & Grantham-McGregor, S. (2003). A randomized controlled trial of a home-visiting intervention on cognition and behavior in term low birth weight infants. The Journal of Pediatrics, 143, 634-639. doi: https://doi.org/10.1067/S0022-3476(03)00455-4-
dc.relation/*ref*/Gertler, P., Heckman, J., Pinto, R., Zanolini, A., Vermeersch, C., Walker, S., Chang, S. and Grantham-McGregor, S. (2014). Labor Market Returns to an Early Childhood Stimulation Intervention in Jamaica. Science, 344 (6187), 998–1001. doi: 10.1126/science.1251178-
dc.relation/*ref*/Giraldo-Huertas, J., Cano, L. & Pulido-Alvarez, A. (2017). Desarrollo Socio-cognitivo en la primera infancia: los retos por cumplir en Salud Pública en la zona Sabana Centro y Boyacá. Revista de Salúd Pública, 19 (4), 484-490. doi: 10.15446/rsap.v19n4.51787-
dc.relation/*ref*/Gould, S. J. (2004). Estructura de la teoría de la evolución. Barcelona: Tusquets Editores, 2a ed.-
dc.relation/*ref*/Gould, S. J. & Vrba, E. (1982). Exaptation – A Missing Terms in the Science of Form. Paleobiology, 8 (1) ,4-15. Recuperado de: https://faculty.insead.edu/vibha-gaba/documents/Gould%20%26%20Vrba_Exaptation.pdf-
dc.relation/*ref*/Hackworth, N., Berthelsen, D., Matthews, J., Westrupp, E., Cann, W., Ukoumunne, O., … Nicholson, J. (2017). Impact of a Brief Group Intervention to Enhance Parenting and the Home Learning Environment for Children Aged 6–36 Months: a Cluster Randomised Controlled Trial. Preventive Science, 18, 337–349. doi: 10.1007/s11121-017-0753-9-
dc.relation/*ref*/Hess, R., Holloway S., Price G., & Dickson W. (1982). Family environments and the acquisition of reading skills. En: L. Laosa, & I. Sigel, (Eds.). Families as Learning Environments of Children (pp. 87-113). New York: Plenum.-
dc.relation/*ref*/Higgins, E., Ruble, D. & Hartup, W. W. (1983). Social cognición and social development: A sociocultural perspective. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.-
dc.relation/*ref*/Iceta, A. & Yoldi M. (2002). Desarrollo psicomotor del niño y su valoración en atención primaria. Anales del Sistema Sanitario de Navarra, 25(2): 35-43. Recuperado de: https://recyt.fecyt.es/index.php/ASSN/article/viewFile/5531/4546-
dc.relation/*ref*/Ipsos MORI and Nairn, A. (2011). Children’s Well-being in UK, Sweden and Spain: The Role of Inequality and Materialism. A Qualitative Study. London: UNICEF. Recuperado de: https://www.ipsos.com/sites/default/files/publication/1177-03/sri-unicef-role-of-inequality-and-materialism-june-2011.pdf-
dc.relation/*ref*/Johnson, P. & Kossykh, Y. (2008). Early Years, Life Chances and Equality: A literature review. Frontier Economics, Research report 7. Equality and Human Rights Commission. Recuperado de: www.equalityhumanrights.com/uploaded_files/research/7_earlyyears_lifechances.pdf-
dc.relation/*ref*/Kagan, J. (1999) The Role of Parents in Children's Psychological Development. Pediatrics, 104 (1), 164--
dc.relation/*ref*/Recuperado de: https://pediatrics.aappublications.org/lookup/pmidlookup?view=long&pmid=10390284-
dc.relation/*ref*/Kinzler, K. D. y Spelke, E. S. (2007). Core systems in human cognición. Progress in Brain Research, 164, 257-264. doi: 10.1016/S0079-6123(07)64014-X-
dc.relation/*ref*/Lugo‐Gil, J. y Tamis‐LeMonda, C. S. (2008). Family resources and parenting quality: Links to children’s cognitive development across the first 3 years. Child Development, 79(4), 1065-1085. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.2008.01176.x-
dc.relation/*ref*/Melhuish, E. C. (2010). Impact of the home learning environment on child cognitive development: Secondary analysis of data from “Growing Up in Scotland”. Edimburgo: Scottish Government Social Research. Recuperado de: http://eprints.bbk.ac.uk/7492/1/7492.pdf-
dc.relation/*ref*/Melhuish, E. C., Phan, M. B., Sylva, K., Sammons, P., Siraj‐Blatchford, I. y Taggart, B. (2008a). Effects of the home learning environment and preschool center experience upon literacy and numeracy development in early primary school. Journal of Social Issues, 64(1), 95-114. doi:10.1111/j.1540-4560.2008.00550.x-
dc.relation/*ref*/Melhuish, E. C., Sylva, K., Sammons, P., Siraj-Blatchford, I., Taggart, B., Phan, M. y Malin, A. (2008b). Preschool influences on mathematics achievement. Science, 321(5893), 1161-1162. doi:10.1126/science.1158808-
dc.relation/*ref*/Melhuish, E., Sylva, K., Sammons, P., Siraj-Blatchford, I., & Taggart, B. (2001). The Effective Provision of Pre-school Education Project, Technical Paper 7: Social/behavioural and cognitive development at 3-4 years in relation to family background. London: Institute of Education/DfES. Recuperado de: https://dera.ioe.ac.uk/18189/2/SSU-SF-2004-01.pdf-
dc.relation/*ref*/Murray, L., De Pascalis, L., Tomlinson, M., Vally, Z., Dadomo, H., MacLachlan, B., Woodward, C. & Cooper, P.J. (2016). Randomized controlled trial of a book-sharing intervention in a deprived South African community: effects on carer-infant interactions, and their relation to infant cognitive and socio-emotional outcome. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 57(12), 1370-1379. doi: 10.1111/jcpp.12605-
dc.relation/*ref*/Nussbaum, J. (2013). The Communication of Wisdom: The Nature and Impact of Communication and Language Change Across the Life Span. Journal of Language and Social Psychology, 32, 3, 243-260. doi: 10.1177/0261927X12463009-
dc.relation/*ref*/Nussbaum, J., & Worthington, A. (2017). Lifespan and Developmental Considerations in Health and Risk Message Design. Oxford Research Encyclopedia of Communication. Recuperado de: https://oxfordre.com/communication/view/10.1093/acrefore/9780190228613.001.0001/acrefore-9780190228613-e-318-
dc.relation/*ref*/Olson, K. R. y Dweck, C. S. (2008). A blueprint for social cognitive development. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 3(3), 193-202. doi: 10.1111/j.1745-6924.2008.00074.x-
dc.relation/*ref*/Profamilia (2010). Encuesta Nacional de Demografía y Salud ENDS. Recuperado de: http://www.profamilia.org.co/encuestas/Profamilia/Profamilia/-
dc.relation/*ref*/Pezoa, J., Mendive, S., & Strasser, K. (2019). Reading interest and family literacy practices from prekindergarten to kindergarten: Contributions from a cross-lagged analysis. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 47, 284-295. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ecresq.2018.12.014.-
dc.relation/*ref*/Richter, L., Black, M., Britto, P., et al. (2019). Early childhood development: an imperative for action and measurement at scale. BMJ Global Health, 4:i154–i160. doi:10.1136/ bmjgh-2018-001302-
dc.relation/*ref*/Romano, M., & Windsor, K. (2020). Increasing deictic gesture use to support the language development of toddlers from high poverty backgrounds. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, (50) 1, 129-139. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ecresq.2018.12.004.-
dc.relation/*ref*/Rueda-Posada, M., Quiroz-Padilla, M., & Giraldo-Huertas, J. (2018). Characteristics of the Core Knowledge Systems in children from 3 to 6 years of age - Características de los Sistemas Centrales de Conocimiento en niños de 3 a 6 años de edad. Universitas Psychologica, 17(5), 1-14. doi: 10.11144/Javeriana.upsy17-5.cscc-
dc.relation/*ref*/Shah, A. K., Mullainathan, S., & Shafir, E. (2012). Some consequences of having too little. Science, 338, 682–685. doi: 10.1126/science.1222426-
dc.relation/*ref*/Shah, A. K., Shafir, E., & Mullainathan, S. (2015). Scarcity frames value. Psychological Science, 26, 402–412. doi: 10.1177/0956797614563958-
dc.relation/*ref*/Shah, A., Mullainathan, S. & Shafir, E. (2018). An exercise in self-replication: Replicating Shah, Mullainathan, and Shafir (2012). Journal of Economic Psychology, doi: 10.1016/j.joep.2018.12.001-
dc.relation/*ref*/Shah, S., Sobotka, Y. & Chen, M. (2015) Positive parenting practices, health disparities, and developmental progress. Pediatrics, 136 (2), 318-326. doi: 10.1542/peds.2014-3390-
dc.relation/*ref*/Shonkoff, J.P., & Phillips, D. (Eds.) (2000). From Neurons to Neighborhoods: The science of early childhood development. Committee on Integrating the Science of Early Childhood Development. Washington, DC: National Academy Press. Recuperado de: https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/books/NBK225557/-
dc.relation/*ref*/Spelke, E. S. (2000). Core knowledge. American Psychologist, 55(11), 1233-1243. doi: 10.1037/0003-066X.55.11.1233-
dc.rightsDerechos de autor 2020 Corporación Universitaria Iberoamericanaes-ES
dc.sourceRevista Iberoamericana de Psicología; Vol. 13 Núm. 1 (2020): Psicología del Desarrollo: Investigaciones en torno al estudio del ciclo vital humano; 71-83es-ES
dc.source2500-6517-
dc.source2027-1786-
dc.subjectSocio-Cognitive developmenten-US
dc.subjectChildhood adversityen-US
dc.subjectDevelopmental modelsen-US
dc.subjectPsychology of Scarcityen-US
dc.subjectDesarrollo socio-cognitivoes-ES
dc.subjectAdversidad infantiles-ES
dc.subjectModelos de desarrolloes-ES
dc.subjectPsicología de la escasezes-ES
dc.subjectDesarrollo tempranoes-ES
dc.subjectCuidadoes-ES
dc.subjectModeloes-ES
dc.subjectDesenvolvimento sócio-cognitivopt-BR
dc.subjectadversidade infantilpt-BR
dc.subjectmodelos de desenvolvimentopt-BR
dc.subjectPsicologia da escassezpt-BR
dc.titleEvolutionary structure of care, socio-cognitive development and daily practicesen-US
dc.titleEstructura evolutiva del cuidado, desarrollo socio-cognitivo y prácticas cotidianases-ES
dc.typeinfo:eu-repo/semantics/article-
dc.typeinfo:eu-repo/semantics/publishedVersion-
dc.typeCuantitativoes-ES
Aparece en las colecciones: Facultad de Educación, Ciencias Humanas y Sociales - Iberoamericana - Cosecha

Ficheros en este ítem:
No hay ficheros asociados a este ítem.


Los ítems de DSpace están protegidos por copyright, con todos los derechos reservados, a menos que se indique lo contrario.